Introduction to Phenotypes

Identifying subtypes of specific disorders is an attractive exercise, as it expands our understanding of the individual’s response to therapy, but it remains attached to the approach based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), which is rooted in behavior and frequently does not predict therapeutic response by any individual within the DSM grouping. Phenotypes are an intermediate step between genetics and behavior. These proposed electroencephalography (EEG) phenotypes are semistable states of neurophysiological function. The author proposes a framework allowing one to describe much of the observed EEG variance with a small number of phenotypical categories. These groupings cut across the DSM categories, and unlike the DSM, the phenotypes predict the individual’s response to therapy, for neurofeedback as well as for medication.

The Concept of Phenotypes

Prior studies using statistical analysis of electroencephalography (EEG) have documented clusters of EEG/quantitative EEG (QEEG) features within psychiatric populations (John, Prichep, & Almas, 1992). Experience over the past 30-plus years with a large number of clinical EEGs and, more recently, decades worth of clinical and research experience with QEEG, as well as a review of the field’s literature, have shown that a limited set of EEG patterns can characterize the majority of EEG variance.

In the perspective I’d like the reader to consider, these proposed EEG/EEG groupings might be considered as phenotypes, based on genetics. There is an indirect linkage between genetics and behavior, with an intermediate step. This intermediate step is one that is based on the expression of the genetics and other factors and constitutes the bridging between the person’s genetics and behavior: the phenotype. These phenotypic EEG/QEEG divergence patterns constitute reliable indices of brain function and predict response to therapy.

It should be noted that these phenotypic patterns are not isomorphic with the established Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) categories, although the phenotypes have powerful implications for therapeutic interventions, both with medication and with neurofeedback. One phenotype may be seen in a wide variety of DSM groupings, from posttraumatic encephalopathy, to affective and attentional related DSM groupings, to many more. These ideas have been published elsewhere (Johnstone, Gunkelman, & Lunt, 2005), although these concepts seem to have especially important perspective implications for this special issue of Biofeedback focusing on subtypes of specific DSM categories.(1)

The very concept of an EEG pattern’s being a subtype of a specific disorder seems foundationally flawed to me because of the lack of specificity of the pattern for the DSM grouping. The theta-beta ratio being increased for age may be a metric that is sensitive to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but the same increase in this ratio also may be seen in a wide variety of other clinical presentations as well as in the absence of ADHD, so the lack of specificity remains a problem. I’d like to invite the reader to transcend the DSM’s limited perspective through the use of phenotypes.

Enhancing Neurofeedback Through the Use of Phenotypes

Behaviorally based neurofeedback interventions have been used with great effectiveness in the hands of good clinicians and practitioners, as evidenced by our field’s ever growing efficacy literature. This efficacy literature is based on actual clinical outcome data and provides support for the rapidly growing list of neurofeedback applications that can claim efficacy based on the jointly adopted Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB)/International Society for Neuronal Regulation (ISNR) efficacy template (La Vaque et al., 2002).

Recently, there have been several good publications detailing the state of the efficacy literature in biofeedback and neurofeedback, including the book by Yucha and Gilbert (2004), Evidence-Based Practice in Biofeedback and Neurofeedback, as well as several white papers on specific disorders, in a series sponsored by the AAPB and ISNR (Monastra et al., 2005; Moss, LaVaque, & Hammond, 2004).

Transcend the DSM Using Phenotypes

The literature on medication response prediction suggests that a phenotypic perspective may help enhance our efficacy (Suffin & Emory, 1995). This was also suggested in the outcome improvement reported by Wright and Gunkelman (1998) when he added the QEEG approach to guide neurofeedback.

The presence of genetically linked EEG patterns provides a solidly data-based set of observations on which to propose an initial list of phenotypic patterns. One EEG pattern with genetic links is the low-voltage fast EEG (Gunkelman, 2001). This low-voltage fast pattern was characterized as a phenotype in a recently published study of phenotypic patterns in alcoholism (Enoch, White, Harris, Rohrbaugh, & Goldman, 2002) and by others who have identified the genetic link to Gene 4’s regulation over gamma-aminobutyric acid receptors (Bierut et al., 2002).

Another EEG pattern with genetic links has been identified in some cases of idiopathic epilepsy (Haug et al., 2003). The paroxysmal epileptiform bursts seen in the EEG in these clinical cases may achieve many hundreds of microvolts, occasionally exceeding 400 to 600 ÌV, with spikes and slow components emerging from a relatively normal background EEG. In a survey of genetic factors in epilepsy, Kaneko, Iwasa, and Okada (2002) showed that the most common human genetic epilepsies display a complex pattern of inheritance and that the identities of the specific genes are largely unknown, despite recent advances in the science of genetics. They also show that the genetic markers of certain types of epilepsy have been identified, such as those with neurodegenerative characteristics and a small number of familial idiopathic epilepsies (Haug et al., 2003). A similar pattern of seizures and EEG phenotype is seen in a group of subjects with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes, found in three children with de novo terminal deletions of the long arm of Chromosome 1q. This suggests that this chromosomal location could be a potential site for a candidate gene (Vaughn, Greenwood, Aylsworth, & Tennison, 1996).

Characterizations linking genomic information, intermediate EEG phenotypes, and behavioral manifestation are likely to have important implications for therapeutics. The International Brain Database (Brain Resource Company) includes the EEG, event related potential, neuropsychological test measures, and genetic testing, thus making it unique in allowing an integrative approach that combines neurophysiology, neuroanatomy, cognition, and genetics. The M.I.N.D. Center at the University of California, Davis, uses the phenomic approach in working with pervasive developmental disorder/autism to avoid treating all clients alike in such a heterogeneous population and to help clarify research on clinical therapeutic effects.

Subtypes Versus Phenotypes

Although many studies have shown subtypes within a DSM-identified disorder, such as the work of Prichep et al. (1993) with obsessive-compulsive disorder and that of Chabot, Merkin, Wood, Davenport, and Serfontein (1996) identifying subgroups of ADHD, these subgroups do not have diagnostic specificity because they can be seen in other disorders. These EEG clusters did, however, predict treatment efficacy using medication in both the ADHD and obsessive-compulsive disorder studies. Suffin and Emory’s (1995) article identified frontal theta in attentional problems, similar to the Chabot et al. findings in ADHD, but the same pattern was also identified in affective disorders. The behavioral grouping did not predict the EEG/QEEG pattern, nor did behavior groupings predict the proper pharmaceutical treatment, although the EEG patterns did predict the effective drug intervention in both studies.

The medication intervention, when clinically based on prospectively identifying the phenotype, was the basis for the highly effective intervention in treatment refractory depression in recent pilot work (S. Suffin, personal communication, 2003) at the Sepulveda Veterans Affairs in Los Angeles. The application of these principles was also at the heart of the doubling of the clinical efficacy in a recent study using QEEG to design the neurofeedback intervention for the attention deficit disorder/ADHD population tested, as compared to behaviorally based neurofeedback interventions (Wright & Gunkelman, 1998).

In an updated and abbreviated review of prior work (Johnstone et al., 2005), the Appendix offers proposed phenotypic patterns as well as a listing of the associated neurofeedback interventions. A reading of the original article is advised for more detail, especially with respect to medication response prediction.

Conclusion: An Introductory Framework for EEG/QEEG Phenotypes

It must be remembered that phenotypes may coexist, and the various combinations are too variable to be handled in this limited presentation. This appendix should not be construed as a replacement for professional assistance in designing a neurofeedback intervention, nor in any way can this be used to fully characterize an individual’s EEG/QEEG.

Appendix: Electroencephalography (EEG)/Quantitative EEG Phenotypes

Candidate Phenotype EEG Findings Associated Neurofeedback Approach
Low-voltage fast Low-voltage EEG overall Reward alpha activity posteriorly
Epileptiform Transient spike/wave, sharp waves, paroxysmal EEG Inhibit low and high frequencies; sensorimotor rhythm training; also consider slow cortical potential control
Diffuse slow activity (with or without lower alpha) Increased delta and theta (1-7 Hz) with or without slower posterior alpha Inhibit midline fronto-central activity slower than 10 Hz, add reward for anterior beta for increased stimulating effect
Focal abnormalities (not epileptiform) Focal slow activity or focal lack of activity Inhibit slower activity and reward higher frequencies (consider medical referral)
Mixed fast and slow Increased slower activity, lack of organized alpha, increased beta Inhibit slow frequencies, reward alpha and SMR, inhibit faster beta
Frontal lobe hypoperfusion disturbances Frontally dominant excess theta or alpha frequency activity Inhibit midline fronto-central activity below 10Hz, reward anterior beta for increased effect
Frontal asymmetries Frontal asymmetry primarily measured at F3, F4 Adjust frontal symmetry with alpha, theta, and beta
Excess temporal lobe alpha Increased alpha activity generated in temporal lobe Inhibit alpha over affected temporal region(s),and inhibit frontal slow activity
Faster alpha variants, not low voltage Alpha peak frequency greater than 12 Hz over posterior and parietal cortex Reward 8-10 Hz alpha at Pz, shift alpha frequency slower with alpha/theta protocol
Spindling excessive beta Rhythmic beta with a spindle morphology, often with an anterior prominence Inhibit beta’s spindle frequencies, wide band inhibit; alpha-theta training may help
Persistent eyes-open alpha Alpha doesn’t attenuate by at least 50% with eyes open; it is generally slower alpha Reward beta frequencies, inhibit alpha; reward higher frequency alpha

Note

1. The material is adapted from Johnstone, Gunkelman, and Lunt (2005), Clinical database development: Characterization of EEG phenotypes. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 2, 99-107, and is used with the permission of Clinical EEG and Neuroscience.

References

Bierut, L. J., Saccone, N. L., Rice, J. P., Goate, A., Foroud, T., Edenberg, H., et al. (2002). Defining alcohol-related phenotypes in humans. The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism. Alcohol Research and Health, 26, 208-213.

Chabot, R. J., Merkin, H., Wood, L. M., Davenport, T. L., & Serfontein, G. (1996). Sensitivity and specificity of QEEG in children with attention deficit or specific developmental learning disorders. Clinical EEG, 27, 26-34.

Chabot, R. J., & Serfontein, G. (1996). Quantitative electroencephalographic
profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 951-963.

Enoch, M. A., White, K. V., Harris, C. R., Rohrbaugh, J. W., & Goldman, D. (2002). The relationship between two intermediate phenotypes for alcoholism: Low voltage alpha EEG and low P300 ERP amplitude. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 509-517.

Gunkelman, J. (1998). Evaluating the frontal lobes in affective and attentional disorders with QEEG and EP—the electrophysiology of frontal lobe disconnection syndrome: Implications for neurotherapy. Journal of Neurotherapy, 3(1-2).

Gunkelman, J. (2001). Low voltage or absolute power. Journal of Neurotherapy, 5(1-2), 107-110.

Haug, K., Warnstedt, M., Alekov, A. K., Sander, T., Ramirez, A., Poser, B., et al. (2003). Mutations in CLCN2 encoding a voltage-gated chloride channel are associated with idiopathic generalized epilepsies. Nature Genetics, 33, 527.532.

John, E. R., Prichep, L. S., & Almas, M. (1992). Subtyping of psychiatric patients by cluster analysis of QEEG. Brain Topography, 4, 321-326.

Johnstone, J. (2005a, September). QEEG patterns: Subgroups, profiles, phenotypes. Paper presented at the 13th annual meeting of the International Society for Neuronal Regulation, Denver, CO.

Johnstone, J. (2005b, April). Symposium on quantitative EEG, neurofeedback, and cognition: Quantitative EEG profile as phenotype. Paper presented to the Neuropsychiatric Institute, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA.

Johnstone, J., Gunkelman, J., & Lunt, J. (2005). Clinical database development: Characterization of EEG phenotypes. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 2, 99-107.

Kaneko, S., Iwasa, H., & Okada, M. (2002). Genetic identifiers of epilepsy. Epilepsia, 43(Suppl. 9), 16-20.

La Vaque, T. J., Hammond, D. C., Trudeau, D., Monastra, V., Perry, P., & Lehrer, P. (2002). Template for developing guidelines for the evaluation of the clinical efficacy of psychophysiological interventions. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 27, 273-281.

Monastra, V., Lynn, S., Linden, M., Lubar, J. F., Gruzelier, J., & Vaque,T.J.(2005). Electroencephalographic biofeedback in the treatment of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 30(2), 95-114.

Moss, D., LaVaque, T. J., & Hammond, D. C. (2004). Introduction to white paper series—Guest editorial. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 19, 151-152.

Niedermeyer, E., & Lopes da Silva, F. (1993). EEG patterns and genetics. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva, Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications and related fields (3rd ed., pp. 192195). Baltimore: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins.

Prichep, L. S., & John, E. R. (1992). QEEG profiles of psychiatric disorders. Brain Topography, 4, 249-257.

Prichep, L. S., Mas, F., Hollander, E., Liebowitz, M., John, E. R., Almas, M., et al. (1993). Quantitative electroencephalographic subtyping of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 50(1), 25-32.

Suffin, S. C., & Emory, W. H. (1995). Neurometric subgroups in attentional and affective disorders and their association with pharmacotherapeutic outcome. Clinical Electroencephalography, 26, 76-83.

Vaughn, B. V., Greenwood, R. S., Aylsworth, A. S., & Tennison, M. B. (1996). Similarities of EEG and seizures in del(1q) and benign rolandic epilepsy. Pediatric Neurology, 15, 261-264.

Wright, C., & Gunkelman, J. (1998, September). QEEG evaluation doubles the rate of clinical success: Series data and case studies. Abstract presented at the sixth annual conference of the Society for the Study of Neuronal Regulation, Austin, TX.

Yucha, C., & Gilbert, C. (2004). Evidence-based practice in biofeedback and neurofeedback. Wheat Ridge, CO: Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback.

Biofeedback Association for Applied Psychophysiology & Biofeedback
Volume 34, Issue 3, pp. 95-98 www.aapb.org

8 thoughts on “Introduction to Phenotypes”

  1. I am please to see such a site and my great hope is that the content discussion and posts will remain collegial, research based and on topic.
    Regards,
    Dave

  2. We just finished the editorial review process and have a paper in press in the Journal of Integrative Neuroscience where we predict stimulant responsiveness in ADHD using the phenotype approach. This is work with Martijn Arns and Rein Bretler in Holland.

    We just had a paper on the outcome of addiction treatment guided by phenotype classification accepted in BIOFEEDBACK, for publication over the holidays.

    I have a trip in November to complete a study of depressive patients and controls, and the medication responses will have the blind eliminated after the phenotyope classification.

    I look forward to evidence based discussions of EEG and NF, and to people aggressively addressing their work’s outcomes with well designed studies.

    Jay

  3. Very useful, Jay, especially the information about the low voltage, fast Hz phenotype, which I’ve seen in some PTSD and alcohol abuse clients.

    I predominantly use LENS, but I have wondered whether the above recommended “traditional” NFB approach might have better efficacy for some of these folks.

  4. Ed,

    The addiction papaer that I did with Curtis Cripe’s Crossroads Institute providing the therapy showed 2/3 of thgose with addiction to have over-arousal as a drive mechanism, with the other 1/3 havinbg a cingulate drive (obsessive/compulsive). The over-arousal group had either a low voltage fast variant, or a beta spoindle excess, or a faster alpha frequency variant… all associated with over-arousal, and all having some form of alpha/theta work (some with pre-treatment).

    I’m looking forward to the publication of the paper, as the neuro-cognitive testing we did also shows the tremendous improvement in cognitive function with the training. It would almost look like the training was a peak performance oriented therapy if you didn’t know the underlying facts of the therapy.

    Phenotype directed training doesn;t look at diagnosis… only at prediction of effective therapy approaches.

    Jay Gunkelman

  5. This is a great website. It is very good to see Jay’s article here. A patient had told me about the video clip of the Indian woman but I had not seen it. Thanks for the posting. Look forward to more on this site.
    Riki Rager
    Virtual Therapies of Alabama

  6. BTW…I think it’s really cool that you found a site banner that had the relatively rare picture of a brain with Broca’s Area on the right hemisphere.

  7. Thanks for the article Jay. Since I am doing SCP-training I wonder if it would be okay to train SCPs over sites of excess temporal alpha. I figure that this will increase negativity and make this area faster and “turn it on”.
    It would be really nice to hear from you and other people out there with more experience.
    Greetings
    Edith

  8. Edith,

    Thanks for the post.

    Increasing basal membrane negativity with SCP training would have correlations with EEG changes in the beta and gamma ranges reliably, as the area is turned on… however engaing the area in function is needed to reduce alpha “idling”, even when cortex is “on”.

    Thats said, having the area able to be active by being “On” is needed prior to engaging the area functionally, so SCP training is a good start!

    Jay

Leave a Comment